Bonobos, one of our closest living animal relatives, show humanlike ability to work together outside social borders in new study

It turns out that one of our closest living relatives, bonobos, are also able to think outside the group.

These were the findings of a Harvard study that involved two years of data collection in the deep forests of the Democratic Republic of Congo, where up to 20,000 of the endangered apes make their only home. The ability to engage in such “out-group” cooperation is the foundation on which humans have created societies and cultures through trade and knowledge-sharing.

“Our work with bonobos is showing that cooperation beyond social borders, without immediate payoff between unrelated individuals, is not uniquely human,” said senior author Martin Surbeck, assistant professor in the Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, who has researched bonobos for 20 years. The study was published in Science with lead author Liran Samuni, a former Harvard research associate who works at the German Primate Center in Göttingen.

A previous study, based on the same bonobo communities, found that the primates maintained distinct, stable social borders, so called “communities.” In their latest analysis, Samuni and Surbeck found evidence of cooperation between members of different communities, facilitated by an assortment of key individuals. These certain few consistently engaged in behaviors such as grooming and food-sharing and acted as links between groups — think ape ambassadors. Within each behavior, individuals cooperated with specific counterparts who were also good cooperators in that domain.

Chat:

How did cooperation evolve among insects and animals more broadly?

Cooperation among insects and animals has evolved over millions of years through the process of natural selection. Cooperative behavior has been favored because it can increase the survival and reproductive success of individuals within a group.

In insects, social cooperation has evolved in a variety of ways. For example, ants, bees, and wasps live in highly organized societies where individuals have specific roles and work together to achieve common goals. This is known as eusociality, and it has evolved multiple times in different insect lineages.

In other cases, cooperation among insects may be more limited. For example, some species of beetles will defend a shared food source from competitors, but they do not form long-lasting social groups.

Cooperation also occurs among animals more broadly, including mammals, birds, and fish. For example, many bird species will engage in cooperative breeding, where multiple adults help care for the young of a single breeding pair. Similarly, many mammal species, such as wolves and meerkats, live in social groups where individuals cooperate to defend territory, hunt, and care for young.

The evolution of cooperation is often facilitated by kin selection, where individuals are more likely to help their relatives because they share some of their genes. This can be seen in the eusocial insects, where individuals are often closely related to their nestmates. However, cooperation can also evolve among unrelated individuals if there are benefits to working together, such as in cooperative hunting or foraging.

Overall, the evolution of cooperation among insects and animals is a complex and fascinating topic that has been studied extensively by biologists and evolutionary theorists.

Leave a Reply